- Home
- Adrian Owen
Into the Gray Zone Page 2
Into the Gray Zone Read online
Page 2
The histories and stories of what had happened to these patients once their brains had been tampered with fascinated me. One patient I worked with had minimal frontal-lobe damage but became wildly disinhibited as a result. Before his injury he was described as a “shy and intelligent young man.” Postinjury he abused strangers in the street and carried a canister of paint with him to deface any public or private surface he could get his hands on. His speech was littered with expletives. His wild behavior escalated: he persuaded a friend to hold his ankles while he hung from the window of a speeding train, a lunatic activity by any measure. His skull and most of the front part of his cortex were crushed when he crashed headlong into a bridge. By some circular twist of fate, his minor frontal-lobe injury led directly to major damage to the same part of his brain.
Perhaps the most bizarre case I encountered concerned a young man with “automatisms”—brief unconscious behaviors during which you are unaware of your actions. Automatisms are typically caused by epileptic seizures that start in the temporal or frontal lobes and then quickly spread—an escalating cascade of neuronal firing that engulfs the entire brain. During these episodes, patients hang in a kind of gray zone. Their eyes remain open, and they are strangely animate and seemingly purposeful in their actions. These usually include routine activities: cooking, showering, or driving a familiar route. Following the episode, the patient regains consciousness and often feels disoriented but has no memory of the event.
My patient was a lanky youth with wild hair whom I tested for memory impairments following surgery that he had received to combat seizures. He was also the defendant in a murder trial. The victim was his own mother, strangled while she was securely locked in the house with her son. Just the two of them. The case turned on his being a martial arts expert with a history of epileptic automatisms, and he could (although the evidence remained entirely circumstantial) have killed her through a series of routine martial arts maneuvers and remained entirely unaware of this dreadful act.
When I assessed his memory using what were then our state-of-the-art computerized tests, I sat near the door—a strategy I had seen in numerous TV crime dramas. I didn’t feel safe. I needed a weapon. All this now seems ludicrous, but there I was, sitting in a closed office with a man who was accused of killing his own mother with his bare hands without even knowing that he’d done it! If he had done it, could he be judged responsible? I wasn’t sure. The thinking then and now was that automatisms, rather than expressing subconscious impulses, are automatic programs firing in the brain, completely outside our control. If he had been a carpenter, he would have been sawing a piece of wood rather than karate-chopping his mom.
Could his brain make him kill again? That was the uppermost question in my mind. What could I use to defend myself? The office around me was stacked high with papers, books, and the paraphernalia of scientific investigation—not exactly an armory. Beside the desk I spied a squash racquet. I clutched it, mulling over some vague plan to parry the young man’s blows. Fortunately for both of us the session passed without incident. I have often thought what an odd sight it would have been: the patient attacking me like a ninja while I tried to swat him about the head with a squash racquet.
The work was enthralling, but all the while I was losing touch with Maureen. Within a year of buying our apartment, the relationship fell apart. We were going in different directions: me into a career in science and her into a job in psychiatric care. Something had changed between us. I couldn’t understand why she’d lost the sense of shared wonder about the brain and how it is affected by damage and disease. I couldn’t understand the appeal of what felt like simply caring for a problem rather than trying to solve it. I’d made the decision, some years earlier, not to pursue a traditional medical career. I’d never wanted to be a physician, listening to people’s ailments and dishing out medication according to standard protocols. I wanted to try to understand the mysteries of the way our minds work and perhaps discover new approaches to treatment and cures. That’s what neuroscientists do. I thought that I had my eye on the bigger picture, but I was probably just insufferably self-righteous, driven by the ambition and idealism of a young scientist. I thought we might be able to understand and then cure Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s diseases.
I was also dazzled by what then impressed me in my naïveté as the glamour that a high-flying career in neuroscience might offer. My boss was sending me to exotic locales to give talks in his stead. At an academic conference in Phoenix, Arizona, I found myself in a hot tub in the desert with two other English neuroscientists. Can you imagine? The day before we had all been plodding through the perpetual precipitation and dreariness of England, and then there we were, luxuriating among the cacti.
I must have been a bit smug when I came home from these trips. Maureen and I had a running argument about the rights and wrongs of psychiatric care, science for science’s sake, and the innate tensions between scientific discovery and medical care.
“It’s all very well studying these people,” I remember Maureen saying. “But helping them deal with their problems is a much better use of resources.”
“If we don’t do the science, these problems will persist!” I countered.
“Science might help someone down the line, years from now. But it mostly comes to nothing. And it doesn’t help patients who donate their time to your research projects, naively assuming that you are going to make their lives better.”
“I do tell them that my research is not going to help them personally.”
“Wow. Aren’t you nice?”
Our running argument had undertones of England versus Scotland. Since the beginning of time, the Scots have felt exploited by the English, whom they see as cold, bloodless mercenaries while they are passionate, earthly, and honest. In retrospect, our care-versus-pure-science positions echoed this age-old conflict.
Eventually, I met someone else and I left Maureen, moving out in 1990 just as the UK economy and housing market collapsed. Our £60,000 apartment was suddenly worth £30,000. We had an enormous negative equity. The interest rate on our mortgage doubled, which was barely manageable while Maureen lived in the apartment. Things rapidly deteriorated when she also moved in with someone else. To make the mortgage payments we were forced to rent the apartment to Brazilian friends, but Maureen wanted nothing more to do with it. I collected rent, paid the mortgage, and took care of taxes and repairs. Maureen and I were no longer on speaking terms—just sending angry letters back and forth. I ended up sleeping on the floor of a friend’s apartment in North London, a whole hour’s drive through rush-hour traffic to see my patients at the Maudsley Hospital. The previous owners had taken their cats but left the fleas. It was a miserable time.
That same year, as I went from patient to patient in South London documenting their brain injuries and their stories, strange things started to happen to my own mother’s health. She began experiencing blinding headaches and behaved in odd ways. One afternoon she disappeared for several hours and upon her return explained that she had been to see a film at the local theater. She hadn’t been to the movies in years and certainly not on her own in the middle of the day. She had just turned fifty, and our family doctor concluded that her menopause was to blame, both for her headaches and curious, unusual excursions. He couldn’t have been more wrong. One evening at home as she watched TV with my father, it became more clear that something was seriously amiss.
“What do you think of the woman’s dress?” my father asked, referring to a woman on the far left side of the screen.
“What woman?” My mother couldn’t see the woman. In fact, she couldn’t see anything in her left visual field at all.
Whatever was causing her headaches and odd behavior was now also affecting her vision. Simple tasks, such as crossing the street, became too dangerous for her to tackle alone. Imagine that you are no longer able to see anything in one part of your visual field (what you see from left to right as you look straight ahead). The problem is that our brains are remarkably good at adapting to change, and in situations such as this, they can literally reconfigure our worldview to what can be seen, completely ignoring what can’t. The missing part does not appear as empty space or as blackness, as one might imagine—it ceases to appear at all. Crossing the road with no awareness of anything on her left side was no longer something that we were going to let my mother attempt alone.
A CT scan revealed that my mother had an oligoastrocytoma growing inside her brain—a cancerous tumor that was pushing its way into the folds of her cortex, interfering with her behavior, affecting her moods, changing how she saw the world, and altering her whole sense of being. We were all devastated. Suddenly, my family’s life and my chosen career were colliding in the most diabolical way imaginable. If she’d been sent for surgery and lost part of her brain as a result, my mother could easily have ended up as a patient in one of my research studies. It was a nightmarish thought.
I was now on the other side of the fence. No longer the detached young scientist but a distraught family member—a situation I’d seen many times among the patients and families that I had been visiting in and around South London. Unfortunately, unlike the tumors in many of those patients, my mother’s was deemed inoperable, and she began round after round of chemotherapy, radiotherapy, and steroid treatment. Swelling around a brain tumor puts pressure on surrounding tissue—that’s what causes the headaches. Steroids reduce the swelling and relieve those symptoms. My mother’s hair fell out and she became bloated (a frequent side effect of steroids).
Fortunately for my family, my sister had qualified as a nurse in 1990 and had been working at the Royal Marsden Hospital, a famous London institution that is dedicated to cancer diagnosis, treatment, research, and education. She gave up work in July of 1992 to care for my mother at our family home. That same month I submitted my PhD thesis, which told the stories of patients with brain disorders, including tumors similar to the one my mother was battling. Before I could formally graduate, I had to defend my thesis, and that would take some months to arrange. By then it was clear that my mother would soon die. I desperately wanted her to see me graduate with a PhD. I called the main administrative office at the University of London and explained the circumstances. Without hesitation they agreed to let me “graduate” despite my not yet having competed the full requirements of the PhD—that would come later. We never told my mother. She was at my graduation, although she may not have been aware of what was going on. I vividly remember my father and I hauling her out of her wheelchair into one of the seats in the auditorium, me dressed in my flowing graduation gown, her in the best clothes we could find that still fit her. We lost our grip and she fell helplessly into the aisle. These are the consequences of progressive brain damage that no one tells you about. In between what you once were and what you eventually become is a grueling adaptation to the deterioration of your day-to-day abilities as tasks become increasingly difficult and finally impossible.
Soon after graduation day, my mother slipped into her own gray zone, not quite there, but not quite gone. Still living at home, now bedridden in the ground-floor dining room since she could no longer climb stairs, she slipped in and out of consciousness from the massive doses of painkillers and sedatives administered by our family doctor. Sometimes she recognized us, sometimes she didn’t. Sometimes she was lucid, sometimes she made no sense at all. My brother flew home from the States, where he was in the throes of his own postdoctoral studies at NASA’s Goddard Space Flight Center in Maryland, and we spent the last few days together as a family. She died in the early hours of the morning on November 15, 1992. We were all at her bedside when she finally stopped breathing.
Many dark days followed, but in a strange way something good came of my mother’s death. After four years of meeting those affected by brain damage and documenting their lives, I got to be on the other side and experience what it is like to watch someone you love get slowly drawn into the abyss. Whether that experience made me even more determined to pursue a career in brain research I do not know, but it certainly prepared me for the many encounters I would have with brain-injured patients and their families in the years to come. I knew firsthand what they were going through, and I felt for them. I wanted to help in any way I could.
Shortly before my mother’s death, I had been offered a postdoctoral position in Montreal, Canada, and now I jumped at the chance to move abroad. I was more than ready to walk away from the ruinous apartment and failed relationship with Maureen and my mother’s death from a brain tumor at fifty. I was through with England and took a three-year position at the Montreal Neurological Institute.
Arriving at “the Neuro” at the end of 1992 to work with Michael Petrides, then the head of the Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, was a significant slice of good fortune. Michael was passionate about brain anatomy and always keen to embrace any new approach or method that might help illuminate how the brain does mental activities such as memory, attention, and planning. Over the next three years, we spent many hours poring over his drawings of the frontal lobes, scribbling little notes about what each area of the brain probably did and designing new tests that would show us how different parts of the brain contributed to memory. I would go away and program them on my IBM 386—state-of-the-art then but woefully underpowered by today’s standards.
This was the year that what were called positron-emission tomography (PET) “activation studies” took off, driven, in part, by developments in the computing industry that allowed us to capture large data sets and digital images of the brain in action. From the launch of the Hubble Space Telescope and the Human Genome Project, computers were revolutionizing every aspect of science. And we were part of that revolution.
Volunteers for PET activation studies would lie in the scanner and be injected with small amounts of a radioactive tracer, and then we’d ask them to perform a task: remembering an unfamiliar face we flashed in front of them, for example. The principle was delightfully simple: those parts of the brain that were working hardest required more oxygen, which was delivered in the blood. Blood flow increased to areas involved in a task. We could literally map the movement of blood around the brain with our PET scanner.
It was a neuropsychologist’s dream come true. No longer would we have to wait for a special patient to come through the door with damage to one specific part of the brain in order to deduce what that brain area did. Now we could simply put healthy people in the scanner and ask them to perform our cognitive tests while we watched their brains spring to life and reach the very same conclusions.
Much of the early work was confirmatory, but that just added to the excitement. For instance, we’d known for some years that the fusiform gyrus, an area on the undersurface of the brain, is involved in face recognition; patients with damage to that area have problems recognizing people they know, a condition known as prosopagnosia, or “face blindness.” But to see the ultimate confirmation of this, when this area lit up in a group of healthy participants as they looked at a series of familiar faces presented on the computer screen, was astounding.
We naively thought we were going to be able to quickly unlock all the secrets of the brain, PET scan by PET scan; but we soon ran into the limitations of what we had at first thought was limitless technology. First among them was the so-called radiation burden. For each scan we gave participants a safe but significant dose of radioactivity. This limited the number of scans we could give any one person, which seriously restricted how many scientific questions we could ask in any one study.
The second problem with PET was that the changes in blood flow that we detected were so small that it was virtually impossible to identify them with a single scan. We had to repeat scans to build a clear picture of what was happening in the brain. We inevitably hit the radiation burden, sometimes before we’d answered a single scientific question to our satisfaction. The answer was to average the data from multiple participants. Indeed, the signals from the brain were so small that this is what we had to do most of the time.
That posed a third problem—our scientific conclusions were not about individuals but groups. Rarely could we say what a particular part of the brain was doing in any one person. Rather, our conclusions would typically take the form of “On average, across the group . . .”
A fourth limitation of PET was timing. A single scan took between sixty and ninety seconds, and what you saw at the end was the sum total of everything that happened during that period. Individual “events” slipped under the radar. Imagine a task where we asked participants to view and remember a series of faces during a ninety-second scan. It was hard to know whether the brain activity that we’d see after the analysis was complete was caused simply by the seeing of the faces, by the remembering of those faces, by some of the faces and not others . . . the list of unknowns went on and on. In spite of all of these limitations, those of us who studied the brain thought all our Christmases had come at once. From the minute I set foot in the door and began designing PET activation studies I was hooked.
One of my early successes showed that one area of the frontal lobes was crucial for organizing our memories. It wasn’t the place where memories were stored or the part of the brain that committed information to memory. Rather it dictated “how” memory should be organized. Visualize trying to remember where you parked your car this morning in a lot you use each day. How do you remember today’s parking spot and not confuse it with the place where you parked yesterday, or the day before, or last week? You could use a landmark, such as a tree or a nearby building, but you’ve probably used all those landmarks before and you are bound to get confused by them. You have to make a special kind of memory decision—you have to decide that, of all the parking spaces that you have in your memory from days gone by, this is the space that you are going to remember today. You have to label this particular space as special and especially relevant for today. This process is an example of what we call working memory, which is a special kind of memory that we only need to retain for a limited period, until the information is used, in this case until you successfully retrieve your car at the end of the day. Then the whole process starts again the next day.